Please ensure Javascript is enabled for purposes of website accessibility
top of page

Working Group on Chimpanzee Cultures

Bringing together expertise on chimpanzee cultural behaviour, conservation science, and policy to drive multidisciplinary approaches to chimpanzee conservation

ABOUT US

We are a multidisciplinary and international team of researchers and conservationists actively engaged in the conservation of chimpanzees, their habitats, and their cultures. Our members have diverse backgrounds, bringing together academic and practical expertise in chimpanzee behaviour, ecology and archaeology, as well as conservation, social sciences, community engagement, policy and project management. 

The WGCC is part of the International Union for the Conservation of Nature (IUCN) Species Survival Commission (SSC) Primate Specialist Group (PSG) Section on Great Apes (SGA), whose overarching mission is to promote great ape conservation using the current best practices and technical information.

​OUR MISSION

  • BUILD a collaborative network of specialists engaged in promoting, conserving, and researching chimpanzee cultures

  • IDENTIFY the avenues in which chimpanzee culture can be used as an asset for conservation

  • PROVIDE guidance to stakeholders and policy-makers on how chimpanzee culture can be integrated into broader conservation policies

  • DEVELOP a protocol for the integration and assessment of behavioural and cultural diversity in field surveys and population monitoring

  • SUPPORT in-country conservation and research efforts that seek to expand our collective knowledge of chimpanzee cultural diversity

  • PROMOTE multidisciplinary, inclusive, and holistic approaches to chimpanzee conservation that take into account the individuals, the population, its habitat, and its cultural behaviours, while also considering the local human communities and their needs

  • SHARE scientific understanding on chimpanzee culture and its relevance to conservation at local and global scales

Our Team

Piel_Profle_2020a - Alex Piel.png

Alex Piel

University College London

Gaku Ohashi.jpg

Gaku Ohashi

Chubu University

Lydia Luncz - Lydia Luncz.jpg

Dr Lydia V. Luncz

Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology

Cat_Hobaiter.jpg

Catherine Hobaiter

University of St Andrews

Emmanuel Danquah.png

Emmanuel Danquah

Kwame Nkrumah University of Science and Technology

Mamadou Saliou DIALLO.png

Mamadou Saliou Diallo

Guinée Ecologie

CarvalhoFinal.jpg

Susana Carvalho

University of Oxford

Erin Wessling.jpg

Erin Wessling

Harvard University

Tatyana Humle - Tanya Humle.jpg

Tatyana Humle

Re:wild

Kathelijne Koops.JPG

Kathelijne Koops

University of Zurich

Rachel Ashegbofe.jpg

Rachel Ashegbofe

SW/Niger Delta Forest Project

Lir pic - Liran Samuni.JPG

Liran Samuni

Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University

KatWarren_Square.jpg

Katarina Almeida-Warren

University of Oxford

Ekwoge_Abwe.JPG

Ekwoge Abwe

-

Photo - Serge Soiret.jpg

Soiret Serge

Centre Suisse de Recherches Scientifiques en Côte d'ivoire (CSRS)/Université Félix Houphouët-Boigny, Abidjan, Côte d'Ivoire

Kalan.jpg

Ammie Kalan

Department of Anthropology, University of Victoria

Kimberley Hockings.JPG

Kimberley Jane Hockings

Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, UK

Woman Hiking Outdoor

Mimi Arandjelovic

iDiv/MPI-EVA

Artboard-1.jpg

Boesch Christophe

Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology

Pascual-Garrido_1.jpg

Alejandra Pascual-Garrido

University of Oxford

Artboard-1.jpg

Crickette Sanz

Washington University

Chimpanzee Culture 

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes ssp.) are one of our closest living relatives with whom we share a common ancestor that lived around 7-8 million years ago[1]. Currently, there are four recognised subspecies of chimpanzees[2]: Central chimpanzee (P. t. troglodytes); Western chimpanzee (P. t. verus); Nigeria-Cameroon chimpanzee (P. t. ellioti); Eastern chimpanzee (P. t.schweinfurthii). All have extensive and diverse cultural repertoires and traditions[3,4], which researchers are continuing to uncover[5,6].

Cultural behaviours include tool use, vocal dialects, non-verbal communication (e.g., gestures), and feeding behaviours (e.g., what foods they eat and how they obtain them). Many of these behaviours vary between subspecies, and even within populations of the same subspecies. The variety of socially-learned behaviours, that is behaviours learned from others, unique to each chimpanzee society is termed cultural diversity. Many of these behaviours, especially those connected to foraging, allow chimpanzees to be flexible and adapt when faced with changing environmental conditions or disturbances.

Chimpanzees are listed as endangered on the IUCN Red List[2], with the Western chimpanzee (P. t. verus) classified as Critically Endangered[7]. The main threats are habitat destruction, often as a result of agricultural expansion and industrial development, and hunting. These lead to population isolation and decline, which not only affect genetic diversity, but also cultural diversity by severing the social ties that enable the transmission and maintenance of behaviours within a social group. To aid conservation efforts, the preservation of cultural diversity, in tandem with genetic diversity, is vital to ensure that behavioural knowledge is not lost and that chimpanzees have a flexible tool-kit of behaviours to adapt to changing environmental conditions.
 

References

  1. Langergraber, K. E. et al. Generation times in wild chimpanzees and gorillas suggest earlier divergence times in great ape and human evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 109, 15716–15721 (2012).

  2. Humle, T., Maisels, F., Oates, J. F., Plumptre, A. & Williamson, E. A. Pan troglodytes (errata version published in 2018). IUCN Red List Threat. Species e.T15933A129038584 (2016) doi:10.2305/IUCN.UK.2016-2.RLTS.T15933A17964454.en.

  3. Whiten, A. et al. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399, 682–685 (1999).

  4. Whiten, A. et al. Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour 138, 1481–1516 (2001).

  5. Boesch, C. et al. Chimpanzee ethnography reveals unexpected cultural diversity. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 910–916 (2020).

  6. Schuppli, C. & van Schaik, C. P. Animal cultures: how we’ve only seen the tip of the iceberg. Evol. Hum. Sci. 1, e2 (2019).

  7. Humle, T. et al. Pan troglodytes ssp. verus. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species e.T15935A102327574. http://www.iucnredlist.org/details/15935/0 (2016) doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2016-2.RLTS.T15935A17989872.en.

  8. Bermejo, M. & Illera, G. Tool-set for termite-fishing and honey extraction by wild chimpanzees in the Lossi Forest, Congo. Primates 40, 619–627 (1999).

  9. Sanz, C. M. & Morgan, D. B. Elemental variation in the termite fishing of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Biol. Lett. 7, 634–637 (2011).

  10. Boesch, C., Head, J. S. & Robbins, M. M. Complex tool sets for honey extraction among chimpanzees in Loango National Park, Gabon. J. Hum. Evol. 56, 560–569 (2009).

  11. Luncz, L. V., Mundry, R. & Boesch, C. Evidence for cultural differences between neighboring chimpanzee communities. Curr. Biol. (2012) doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.031.

  12. Bessa, J., Biro, D. & Hockings, K. J. Inter-community behavioural variation confirmed through indirect methods in four neighbouring chimpanzee communities in Cantanhez NP, Guinea-Bissau. R. Soc. Open Sci. 9, (2022).

  13. Pruetz, J. D. & Bertolani, P. Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, hunt with tools. Curr. Biol. 17, 412–417 (2007).

  14. Kühl, H. S. et al. Human impact erodes chimpanzee behavioral diversity. Science 363, 1453–1455 (2019).

  15. Hockings, K. J. & McLennan, M. R. From Forest to Farm: Systematic Review of Cultivar Feeding by Chimpanzees – Management Implications for Wildlife in Anthropogenic Landscapes. PLOS ONE 7, e33391 (2012).

  16. Hockings, K. J., Anderson, J. R. & Matsuzawa, T. Socioecological adaptations by chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, inhabiting an anthropogenically impacted habitat. Anim. Behav. 83, 801–810 (2012).

  17. Bryson-Morrison, N., Matsuzawa, T. & Humle, T. Chimpanzees in an anthropogenic landscape: Examining food resources across habitat types at Bossou, Guinea, West Africa. Am. J. Primatol. 78, 1237–1249 (2016).

  18. Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L. & Boesch, C. Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: A case for vocal learning? Ethology 110, 221–243 (2004).

  19. Mitani, J. C., Hasegawa, T., Gros-Louis, J., Marler, P. & Byrne, R. W. Dialects in wild chimpanzees? Am. J. Primatol. 27, 233–243 (1992).

  20. Mitani, J. C., Hunley, K. L. & Murdoch, M. E. Geographic Variation in the Calls of Wild Chimpanzees: A Reassessment. Am. J. Primatol. 47, 133–151 (1999).

  21. McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., Scott, S. E. & Tutin, C. E. G. Intergroup Differences in a Social Custom of Wild Chimpanzees: The Grooming Hand‐Clasp of the Mahale Mountains. Curr. Anthropol. 42, 148–153 (2001).

  22. Sugiyama, Y. Observations on the population dynamics and behavior of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea, in 1979–1980. Primates 22, 435–444 (1981).

  23. Luncz, L. V. & Boesch, C. The extent of cultural variation between adjacent chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) communities; A microecological approach. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 156, 67–75 (2015).

  24. Reynolds, V. Diet and culture at Sonso. in The Chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest: Ecology, Behaviour and Conservation (Oxford University Press, 2005). doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198515463.003.0005.

  25. Sept, J. M. & Brooks, G. E. Reports of Chimpanzee Natural History, Including Tool Use, in 16th-and 17th-Century Sierra Leone. Int. J. Primatol. 15, (1994).

  26. Mercader, J. et al. 4,300-year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 104, 3043–3048 (2007).

  27. Koops, K., McGrew, W. C. & Matsuzawa, T. Ecology of culture: do environmental factors influence foraging tool use in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus? Anim. Behav. 85, 175–185 (2013).

  28. Koops, K., Visalberghi, E. & van Schaik, C. P. The ecology of primate material culture. Biol. Lett. 10, 20140508 (2014).

  29. Grund, C., Neumann, C., Zuberbühler, K. & Gruber, T. Necessity creates opportunities for chimpanzee tool use. Behav. Ecol. 30, 1136–1144 (2019).

  30. Hobaiter, C., Poisot, T., Zuberbühler, K., Hoppitt, W. & Gruber, T. Social Network Analysis Shows Direct Evidence for Social Transmission of Tool Use in Wild Chimpanzees. PLoS Biol. 12, e1001960 (2014).

  31. Wrangham, R. W. et al. Distribution of a Chimpanzee Social Custom Is Explained by Matrilineal Relationship Rather Than Conformity. Curr. Biol. 26, (2016).

  32. Luncz, L. V., Sirianni, G., Mundry, R. & Boesch, C. Costly culture: differences in nut-cracking efficiency between wild chimpanzee groups. Anim. Behav. 137, 63–73 (2018).

  33. Lamon, N., Neumann, C., Gier, J., Zuberbühler, K. & Gruber, T. Wild chimpanzees select tool material based on efficiency and knowledge. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 285, 20181715 (2018).

  34. Kalan, A. K. et al. Environmental variability supports chimpanzee behavioural diversity. Nat. Commun. 11, 4451 (2020).

  35. Dijk, K. van, Cibot, M. & McLennan, M. R. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) adapt their nesting behavior after large-scale forest clearance and community decline. Am. J. Primatol. e23323 (2021) doi:10.1002/AJP.23323.

  36. McCarthy, M. S., Lester, J. D. & Stanford, C. B. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Flexibly Use Introduced Species for Nesting and Bark Feeding in a Human-Dominated Habitat. Int. J. Primatol. 38, 321–337 (2017).

  37. Hockings, K. J. et al. Apes in the Anthropocene: Flexibility and survival. Trends Ecol. Evol. 30, 215–222 (2015).
     

Get In Touch:

bottom of page